Recent evidence has shown that suffixes are identified within nonwords only when they follow an existing stem (e.g., in 'shootment', but not in 'mentshoot'), thus suggesting that their mental representation is position‑locked (Crepaldi, Rastle & Davis, 2010). These results rise the important issue of morpheme position coding, which has always been neglected in morphological research; although it is clear that morphemes are coded for position in the word identification system (or otherwise we could not distinguish between words like 'overhang' and 'hangover'), even the most recent theoretical attempts (e.g., Crepaldi, Rastle, Coltheart & Nickels, 2010; Taft, 2006) have ignored this issue. In this study we ask whether stem and prefix identification is sensitive to positional constraints as suffix identification is. To address this question we carried out two lexical decision experiments capitalizing on the well‑established morpheme interference effect as a diagnostic for morpheme identification (e.g., Taft & Forster, 1975); this effect refers to the fact that nonwords comprising existing morphemes (e.g., 'shootment') are rejected more slowly than nonwords that do not have a morphological structure (e.g., 'shootmant'). In Experiment 1 we show that the rejection time of reversed compounds (e.g., 'moonhoney') is longer than that of matched control nonwords (e.g., 'moonbasin'). Because it has been shown that non‑morphological transposed‑halves nonwords (e.g., 'dulesche') do not activate the representation of their corresponding words (e.g., 'schedule'), this effect must be morphological in nature; readers must have thus identified 'honey' and 'moon' within 'moonhoney', and the representations of these morphemes must have activated (at least partially) the word 'honeymoon'. This account implies that stem identification is position-independent. In Experiment 2 we show that the rejection time of pseudo‑prefixed nonwords (e.g., 'predrink') is longer than that of matched control nonwords (e.g., 'pledrink'), thus replicating the morpheme interference effect described in previous studies. Crucially, however, we also show that this effect disappears when the relative order of the prefixes and the stems is reversed; nonwords like 'drinkpre' are as difficult to reject as nonwords like 'drinkple', indicating that 'pre' has not been identified as a prefix in these stimuli. These results extend previous findings (that were limited to suffixes) and allow a strong and general conclusion on the position specificity of morpheme representations; stem identification is position‑independent, whereas affix identification is position‑specific. None of the existing morphological theories accounts for this fact. One possibility is that positional constraints arise from the characteristics of the orthographic input; the word identification system would just capture the fact that affixes appear only in given positions within words (prefixes at the beginning, suffixes at the end), whereas free morphemes may occur everywhere (e.g., 'overdress', 'leftover'). Alternatively, the difference between stems and affixes might depend on their morpho‑semantic properties; because stems convey the core meaning of words, the identification system would be more sensitive to their identification.

Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C. (2010). Seeing Stems Everywhere and Being Blind to Affixes: Positional constraints on Morpheme Identification. Intervento presentato a: 2010 Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing (AMLaP), York, UK.

Seeing Stems Everywhere and Being Blind to Affixes: Positional constraints on Morpheme Identification

CREPALDI, DAVIDE;
2010

Abstract

Recent evidence has shown that suffixes are identified within nonwords only when they follow an existing stem (e.g., in 'shootment', but not in 'mentshoot'), thus suggesting that their mental representation is position‑locked (Crepaldi, Rastle & Davis, 2010). These results rise the important issue of morpheme position coding, which has always been neglected in morphological research; although it is clear that morphemes are coded for position in the word identification system (or otherwise we could not distinguish between words like 'overhang' and 'hangover'), even the most recent theoretical attempts (e.g., Crepaldi, Rastle, Coltheart & Nickels, 2010; Taft, 2006) have ignored this issue. In this study we ask whether stem and prefix identification is sensitive to positional constraints as suffix identification is. To address this question we carried out two lexical decision experiments capitalizing on the well‑established morpheme interference effect as a diagnostic for morpheme identification (e.g., Taft & Forster, 1975); this effect refers to the fact that nonwords comprising existing morphemes (e.g., 'shootment') are rejected more slowly than nonwords that do not have a morphological structure (e.g., 'shootmant'). In Experiment 1 we show that the rejection time of reversed compounds (e.g., 'moonhoney') is longer than that of matched control nonwords (e.g., 'moonbasin'). Because it has been shown that non‑morphological transposed‑halves nonwords (e.g., 'dulesche') do not activate the representation of their corresponding words (e.g., 'schedule'), this effect must be morphological in nature; readers must have thus identified 'honey' and 'moon' within 'moonhoney', and the representations of these morphemes must have activated (at least partially) the word 'honeymoon'. This account implies that stem identification is position-independent. In Experiment 2 we show that the rejection time of pseudo‑prefixed nonwords (e.g., 'predrink') is longer than that of matched control nonwords (e.g., 'pledrink'), thus replicating the morpheme interference effect described in previous studies. Crucially, however, we also show that this effect disappears when the relative order of the prefixes and the stems is reversed; nonwords like 'drinkpre' are as difficult to reject as nonwords like 'drinkple', indicating that 'pre' has not been identified as a prefix in these stimuli. These results extend previous findings (that were limited to suffixes) and allow a strong and general conclusion on the position specificity of morpheme representations; stem identification is position‑independent, whereas affix identification is position‑specific. None of the existing morphological theories accounts for this fact. One possibility is that positional constraints arise from the characteristics of the orthographic input; the word identification system would just capture the fact that affixes appear only in given positions within words (prefixes at the beginning, suffixes at the end), whereas free morphemes may occur everywhere (e.g., 'overdress', 'leftover'). Alternatively, the difference between stems and affixes might depend on their morpho‑semantic properties; because stems convey the core meaning of words, the identification system would be more sensitive to their identification.
abstract + poster
Visual word identification; morpheme; position coding; lexical decision; masked priming
English
2010 Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing (AMLaP)
2010
7-set-2010
open
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C. (2010). Seeing Stems Everywhere and Being Blind to Affixes: Positional constraints on Morpheme Identification. Intervento presentato a: 2010 Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing (AMLaP), York, UK.
File in questo prodotto:
File Dimensione Formato  
SeeingStemsEverywhere_Amlap2010.pdf

accesso aperto

Tipologia di allegato: Other attachments
Dimensione 44.94 kB
Formato Adobe PDF
44.94 kB Adobe PDF Visualizza/Apri
Amlap2010.pdf

accesso aperto

Tipologia di allegato: Other attachments
Dimensione 718.01 kB
Formato Adobe PDF
718.01 kB Adobe PDF Visualizza/Apri

I documenti in IRIS sono protetti da copyright e tutti i diritti sono riservati, salvo diversa indicazione.

Utilizza questo identificativo per citare o creare un link a questo documento: https://hdl.handle.net/10281/20484
Citazioni
  • Scopus ND
  • ???jsp.display-item.citation.isi??? ND
Social impact